APPLYING THE PRINCIPLES OF MOTOR LEARNING TO OPTIMIZE REHABILITATION AND ENHANCE PERFORMANCE AFTER ACL INJURY
Written by Alli Gokeler, Germany and Anne Benjaminse, The Netherlands
09-Mar-2020
Category: Sports Rehab

Volume 9 | Targeted Topic - Return to Performance After ACL Reconstruction | 2020
Volume 9 - Targeted Topic - Return to Performance After ACL Reconstruction

– Written by Alli Gokeler, Germany and Anne Benjaminse, The Netherlands

 

INTRODUCTION

Most athletes who wish to continue sports participation after an anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) injury are advised to undergo ACL reconstruction (ACLR)1. Traditionally, rehabilitation programs have mainly focused on restoring symmetry in range of motion, balance, strength and neuromuscular control. For young athletes (<25 years of age) returning to competitive sports involving high intensity jumping and cutting activities, secondary ACL injury rates of 23% have been reported, and these frequently occur early during the return to sport (RTS) period2.

Restoration of symmetry alone is not sufficient to reduce an athletes risk of reinjury. Focus should also be placed on addressing underlying deficits that likely contributed to the primary ACL injury3. In addition, a series of inciting events are likely to occur prior to the actual injury4, and different playing situations provide further complexity. For example, ball possession, position of team mates and actions of opponents all impose different challenges and problems for the athletes to solve5,6. Thus, perceptual capacities play an important role in team and ball sports7 by enahncing perception in rapidly changing environments. Interpreting situational information correctly and efficiently allows them to select the most appropriate response. The impact of these chaotic environments should not be ignored when second ACL injury prevention is the goal. However, components of neurocognitive training are often not addressed in current ACL rehabilitation programs8.

An ACL injury induces neurological changes to the central nervous system (CNS), due to the loss of information from mechanoreceptors, pain and developed motor compensations9. This neuroplastic disruption progresses until altered motor strategies potentially become the norm. Subsequent restoration of baseline function then becomes a fight against maladaptive neuroplasticity developed in the wake of altered CNS input and motor output compensations10. Rehabilitation should therefore also consider central neurological (brain) drivers of control and ultimately these strategies should be incorporated into secondary injury prevention programs11. Considering the high re-injury rates, current approaches may not be effective in fully targeting residual deficits related to the initial injury and the subsequent surgical intervention12. Furthermore, rehabilitation after ACLR should focus on addressing the underlying neuromuscular control deficits that led to the initial injury and that may be amplified subsequent to ACL injury and reconstruction. The purpose of this article is to present novel clinically integrated motor learning principles to support neuroplasticity that can improve patient functional performance and reduce the risk of second ACL injury.

 

WHAT IS MOTOR LEARNING?

Motor learning is defined as the process of an individual’s ability to acquire motor skills with a relatively permanent change as a function of practice or experience13. Currently the most used method to test motor learning is to assess the behavioral resultant outcome13. To assure that motor learning takes place, a skill must be rehearsed repeatedly. However, there are many variables to consider when planning and structuring practice. Even for commonly used factors such as instructions and feedback, clinicians should be cognizant of the effects that the amount, type, and schedule of instructions respectively can have on long term skill retention.  

 

Optimal practice should ensure long-term learning and this is measured by retention and transfer of skills. In addition, task-specific practice should be used that is meaningful to the patient

 

Further, it is important to ensure the task being practiced is meaningful, challenging and motivating for the patient. Examples of important influencing factors to support the motor learning processes during ACL rehabilitation will be described in this article.

 

ATTENTIONAL FOCUS

In almost any training situation where motor skills are to be learned, patients receive instructions about the correct movement pattern14. Is is important to be aware that instructional language has an influential role on motor learning outcomes15. Typically, a physical therapist instructs the patient to flex the knee more when landing16 (see Figure 1). Instructions that direct the patient’s attention to their own movements induce an internal focus of attention17.

 

It has been shown that 95% of physical therapists provide instructions with an internal focus18. However, a growing body of evidence shows that this type of attentional focus may not be as effective as previously thought19.

 

Interestingly, a simple change in the wording of instructions can have a significant impact on performance and learning. For example, directing the patient’s attention to the effects of the movements on the environment promotes an external focus of attention. In this case, to increase knee flexion when landing from a jump (promoting a soft landing), a physical therapist instructs the patient to touch cones when landing (Figure 1)16. A focus on the movement effect promotes the utilization of unconscious or automatic processes, whereas an internal focus directs attention to one’s own movements results in a more conscious type of control that constrains the motor system and disrupts automatic control processes20. Wulf et al. have termed this the ‘constrained-action hypothesis’ as the explanation for the attentional focus phenomenon21. Support for this view comes from studies showing reduced attentional demands when performers adopt an external as opposed to an internal focus, as well as a higher frequency of low-amplitude movement adjustments, which is seen as an indication of a more automatic, reflex-type mode of control22. The cumulative body of evidence indicates the beneficial effects of using external focus instructions over internal focus instructions22,23.

 

IMPLICIT LEARNING

The aim of implicit learning is to minimize the amount of explicit knowledge about movement execution during the learning process. An example of this would be trying to describe in as few words as possible how you ride a bicycle. Commonly, we have a hard time explaining this using a verbal description. The reason is that we ‘just do it’ (riding a bicycle) and we really don’t need a large pool of conscious detailed knowledge to outline how to execute the movement. If we accept this premise, we can also ask ourselves what we do in a clinical situation with a patient. Commonly, we as clinicians provide a lot of detailed information to the patient. Interestingly, physiotherapists working with children can’t use the explicit instructions and often use implicit learning methods. An example of how implicit learning can be induced is by providing analogies rather than explicit instructions during the acquisition of motor skills24.

 

Analogy, or metaphorical description of the action, connects with a visual image, e.g. ‘pretend you are landing on eggs’, to promote a soft landing.

 

Implicit learning reduces the reliance on the working memory and promotes more of an automatic process24. It is for this reason that it can be more effective in the execution of complex tasks. Competitive sports can be psychologically demanding, and decision-making accuracy deteriorates in athletes when they are under pressure and must deal with increased task complexity25. The negative influences of pressure can be observed in several ways. Of interest in connection to learning is ‘re-investment’, when an athlete begins to direct attention to the skills and movements which should already be automatic, and do not need conscious control. This re-investment may cause the athlete to make sudden mistakes in technical actions, which are relatively simple and have been performed, without error, a thousand times before26.

Explicit learning can promote re-investment because the athlete reverts to memory by a detailed, step-by-step explicit instruction about movement execution. Under stress, an athlete may unwillingly start to follow this guidance and divide smooth and fluent execution into separate blocks that would be detrimental for expert performance (choking). Additionally, such excessive attention to the technical details can draw working memory resources from other aspects of athletic performance24.

One of the most interesting and widely unexplored aspects of implicit learning in rehabilitation is its connection with anticipation and decision making. This may be important in the later stages of rehabilitation when the patient is approaching the RTS phase. An athlete should be progressively exposed to physical, environmental, and psychological stressors that are comparable to those they will experience in the sport they participate in. Considering secondary ACL injury prevention and an increased need to re-establish sports performance skills, training in this phase of the rehabilitation should emphasize motor control factors such as anticipation, responses to perturbation, and visual-motor control within complex task and environmental interactions. Heightened anticipatory and sensorimotor skills obtained through implicit learning may give the athlete an improved capability to anticipate the need for corrective motor actions and avoid potentially high injury-risk scenarios. Evidence of this is present whereby implicit training using limited visual information about the direction of the ball in tennis, improved athletes’ prediction accuracy after the intervention27. Conversely, an explicit learning group, who received specific kinematic information about the tennis serve of the opponent, didn’t demonstrate any improvement in anticipatory skills27.


Progressing the learning challenge through contextual interference

Practice has a key role in the acquisition of motor skills during ACL rehabilitation. When a movement is executed, a learner strengthens his/her motor schemas, storing information about (a) the initial conditions, (b) the response specifications of the motor program, (c) the sensory consequences of the produced response, and (d) the effects of the movement on the environment13. Hence, the way the practice is scheduled influences the acquisition of motor skills. Random, or variable practice involves performing variations of the task or completely different tasks throughout a rehabilitation session. For skill transfer to occur, a review of the literature has suggested that variable practice may be more effective28. Contextual interference in motor learning is defined as the interference in performance and learning that arises from practicing one task in the context of other tasks28. Contextual interference is the effect on learning produced by the order of skills changing across trials. A non-systematic order of skills execution, as well as a non-consecutive execution of the same skill (A-C-B-C-A-B-A-B-C), is observed during random practice. The amount of contextual interference may vary and is generally low in blocked practice and high in random practice. Practicing under conditions of high contextual interference (i.e., with a random practice order) degrades performance during acquisition trials compared with low contextual interference conditions (i.e., with a blocked order, where practice is completed on one task before practice on another task is undertaken)28.

 

While higher contextual interference (random practice) may lead to poor(er) immediate performance, it frequently leads to better learning (as measured by retention and transfer tests) compared with blocked practice29.

 

Clinicians must decide how to best schedule practice to facilitate learning. As mentioned, the skill level of a patient is a factor that may need to be considered in terms of the amount of contextual interference provided29. In general, lower level athletes benefit more from low contextual interference, whereas elite athletes often thrive in learning environments where high levels of contextual interference are present.

 

MOTIVATION

Practice conditions that support fundamental psychological needs such as competence, autonomy, and social relatedness30 appear to create circumstances that enhance motivation and optimize performance and learning31 and further details and practical strategies for each are outlined below.

 

Autonomy

In most rehabilitation situations, clinicians determine the content and specific details of the training session. For example, they decide the order in which tasks are practiced, the duration, and when or if instructions, demonstrations or feedback will be provided. Thus, while clinicians generally control most aspects of practice, patients assume a relatively passive role. Self-controlled learning (where the patient has the choice when to request feedback or may choose an exercise) is a powerful tool in motor learning and has shown advantages in comparison with prescribed training schedules32-34, some examples include:

  1. Encourage the patient to choose three exercises for any given rehabilitation session (while considering variation in level or equipment).
  2. Ask the patient to choose the type of material of a given exercise.
  3. Suggest the patient determines when to receive feedback during selected exercises.

In practice you may provide the patient with three options when practicing a squat:

  1. practicing on a BOSU ball,
  2. practicing in front of mirror with barbell,
  3. practicing as a goblet squat.

When you give the patient the option to chose one of these exercises, in all likelihood, patients will chose what they like best35. This choice is based on:

  1. feelings of competence (‘yes, I can do this!’) or
  2. feeling of relatedness (‘I feel most comfortable / challenged when doing this variation of the exercise’).

Of note, the physiotherapist is still responsible for creating the (safe) environment for learning, offering exercises with similar difficulty level, where a range of materials can be chosen.

 

Positive feedback and autonomy supportive language

It is also important to realise that athletes have a preference to receive positive feed-back. Experiencing competence through feedback on good trials positively affects motor learning through motivational influences such as intrinsic motivation, interest, and enjoyment32,36. For example, providing feedback after good trials (e.g. “That was an excellent jump, you landed very softly’) plays a strong role in confirmation of competence, provides confidence and enhances intrinsic motivation37. Self-controlled feedback schedules also have the potential to help patients become more involved in their learning process by inducing an active role during rehabilitation sessions which subsequently enhances motivation and increases effort and compliance38-40.

Finally, the way in which task instructions are phrased can have a profound influence on learning. Instructions that suggest to learners a certain degree of choice in how they perform a task can promote a more effective learning environment than over utilisation of prescriptive instructions that imply no room for choice. This is very easy to implement through the use of subtle changes in wording. For comparison see the wording in Table 1.

 

SUMMARY

Enhancing movement quality and training transfer are key outcomes of rehabilitation to reduce the risk of reinjury and enhance performance. While the development of physical capacities is of fundamental importance, a large part of the movement solution is grounded in neuromechanics (i.e., the interaction of the brain and muscles to produce coordinated movements in different conditions). Strategies to optimize motor learning can begin early in the rehabilitation process and should continue as the athlete aims to return to sport and optimal performance. Some examples have been outlined in this article including the use of positive, externally focused feedback and increasing levels of contextual interference. However, the optimal solution also needs to be individually tailored to the athlete, and they should also be encouraged to play an active role in their rehabilitation journey. Future research should focus on which combinations of the techniques presented here are most effective for long term skill retention, create the least dependence on external feedback, and provide the greatest transfer to the sporting environment.

 

Alli Gokeler Ph.D.*,**

Post-doctoral researcher

 

Anne Benjaminse Ph.D. **,***

Post-doctoral researcher

 

* Exercise Science and Neuroscience Unit, Department Exercise and Health,

Faculty of Science, University of Paderborn

Paderborn, Germany

 

** Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen,

Groningen, The Netherlands

 

*** School of Sport Studies,

Hanze University Groningen,

Groningen, The Netherlands

 

Contact: alli.gokeler@uni-paderborn.de

 

 

References

 

  1. Marx RG, et al., Beliefs and attitudes of members of the American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons regarding the treatment of anterior cruciate ligament injury. Arthroscopy. 2003; 19: 762-70.
  2. Wiggins AJ, et al. Risk of Secondary Injury in Younger Athletes After Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Am J Sports Med. 2016; 44: 1861-76.
  3. Benjaminse A, Holden S, Myer GD. ACL rupture is a single leg injury but a double leg problem: too much focus on 'symmetry' alone and that's not enough! Br J Sports Med. 2018; 52: 1029-1030.
  4. Bahr R, Krosshaug T. Understanding injury mechanisms: a key component of preventing injuries in sport. Br J Sports Med. 2005; 39: 324-9.
  5. Olsen OE, et al. Injury mechanisms for anterior cruciate ligament injuries in team handball: a systematic video analysis. Am J Sports Med. 2004; 32: 1002-12.
  6. Olsen OE, et al. Injury pattern in youth team handball: a comparison of two prospective registration methods. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2006; 16: 426-32.
  7. Verburgh L, et al. Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS One. 2014; 9: e91254.
  8. Grooms DR, Onate JA. Neuroscience Application to Noncontact Anterior Cruciate Ligament Injury Prevention. Sports Health. 2016; 8: 149-52.
  9. Baumeister J, Reinecke K, Weiss M Changed cortical activity after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction in a joint position paradigm: an EEG study. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2008; 18: 473-84.
  10. Grooms DR, Page SJ, Onate JA. Brain activation for knee movement measured days before second anterior cruciate ligament injury: neuroimaging in musculoskeletal medicine. J Athl Train. 2015; 50: 1005-10.
  11. Grooms DR, Myer GD. Upgraded hardware - What about the software? Brain updates for return to play following ACL reconstruction. Br J Sports Med. 2017; 51: 418-419.
  12. Simoneau GG, Wilk KE. The challenge of return to sports for patients post-ACL reconstruction. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2012; 42: 300-1.
  13. Schmidt RA, Lee T. Motor learning and performance. 5 ed. 2014, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  14. Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013; 6: 77-104.
  15. Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Effortless motor learning? An external focus of attention enhances movement effectiveness and efficiency. In Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in Attention and Action, B. Bruya, Editor. 2010, MIT Press: Cambridge, MA. p. 75-101.
  16. Gokeler A, et al. Feedback techniques to target functional deficits following anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction: implications for motor control and reduction of second injury risk. Sports Med. 2013; 43:1065-74.
  17. Wulf G, Shea CH, Matschiner S. Frequent feedback enhances complex motor skill learning. J Mot Behav. 1998; 30: 180-92.
  18. Durham K, et al. Use of information feedback and attentional focus of feedback in treating the person with a hemiplegic arm. Physiother Res Int. 2009; 14: 77-90.
  19. Wulf G, Shea C, Lewthwaite R. Motor skill learning and performance: a review of influential factors. Med Educ. 2010; 44: 75-84.
  20. Wulf G, McNevin N, Shea CH. The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2001; 54: 1143-54.
  21. Wulf G, Shea C, Park JH. Attention and motor performance: preferences for and advantages of an external focus. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2001; 72: 335-44.
  22. Wulf G, Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2012; 6: 77-104.
  23. Benjaminse A, et al. Novel methods of instruction in ACL injury prevention programs, a systematic review. Phys Ther Sport. 2015; 16: 176-186.
  24. Masters RS, et al. Implicit motor learning and complex decision making in time-constrained environments. J Mot Behav. 2008; 40: 71-79.
  25. Kinrade NP, Jackson RC, Ashford KJ. Reinvestment, task complexity and decision making under pressure in basketball. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2015; 20: 11-19.
  26. Masters R. Knowledge, nerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. Br J Psych. 1992; 83: 343-358.
  27. Farrow D, Abernethy B. Can anticipatory skills be learned through implicit video-based perceptual training? J Sport Sci. 2002; 20: 471-485.
  28. Magill RA, Hall KG. A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Human Movement Science. 1990; 9: 241-289.
  29. Porter JM, Magill RA Systematically increasing contextual interference is beneficial for learning sport skills. J Sports Sci. 2010; 28: 1277-85.
  30. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000; 55: 68-78.
  31. Lewthwaite R, Wulf G. Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill learning. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2010; 63: 738-49.
  32. Benjaminse A, et al. Optimization of the anterior cruciate ligament injury prevention paradigm: novel feedback techniques to enhance motor learning and reduce injury risk. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2015; 45: 170-82.
  33. Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016; 23: 1382-1414.
  34. Benjaminse A, et al. Motor Learning Strategies in Basketball Players and its Implications for ACL Injury Prevention: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc. 2017; 25: 2365-2376.
  35. Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Self-controlled learning: the importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Front Psychol. 2012; 3: 458-466.
  36. Welling W, et al. Enhanced retention of drop vertical jump landing technique: A randomized controlled trial. Hum Mov Sci. 2016; 45: 84-95.
  37. Saemi E, et al. Knowledge of results after relatively good trials enhances self-efficacy and motor learning. Psychol Sport Ex. 2012; 13: 378-382.
  38. Janelle CM, et al. Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1997; 68: 269-79.
  39. Badami R, et al. Feedback after good trials enhances intrinsic motivation. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2011; 82: 360–364.
  40. Andrieux M, Danna J, Thon B. Self-control of task difficulty during training enhances motor learning of a complex coincidence-anticipation task. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2012; 83:  27-35.

 

Figure 1: Internal focus (a) versus external focus (b) instructions during a drop vertical jump. In (a), the patient was instructed to land while bending the hips and knees; in (b), the patient was instructed to touch the cones when landing. Note the increased flexion of trunk, hips and knees (as shown in Figure 1b compared to 1a) indciating a ‘safer’ landing strategy despite the fact that no specific task instructions were provided.
Table 1: Comparison of wording that support autonomy versus wording that is more controlling.

Share

Volume 9 | Targeted Topic - Return to Performance After ACL Reconstruction | 2020
Volume 9 - Targeted Topic - Return to Performance After ACL Reconstruction

More from Aspetar Journal

Sports Science
THE USE OF BLOOD FLOW RESTRICTION IN EARLY STAGE REHABILITAION FOLLOWING ACL INJURY

Written by – Stephen D Patterson, UK, Johnny Owens, USA and Luke Hughes, UK

Sports Science
TAKING A STEP BACK TO RECONSIDER CHANGE OF DIRECTION AND ITS APPLICATION FOLLOWING ACL INJURY

Written by – Philip Graham-Smith, Qatar, Paul Jones, United Kingdom and Paul Read, Qatar

Sports Science
ASSESSING VERTICAL JUMP FORCE-TIME ASYMMETRIES IN ATHLETES WITH ANTERIOR CRUCIATE LIGAMENT INJURY

Written by – Matthew J. Jordan, Graeme Challis, Nathaniel Morris,­ Mike Lane, Jeremiah Barnert and Walter Herzog, Canada

Latest Issue

Download Volume 13 - Targeted Topic - Nerve Compression Syndromes | 2024

Trending

Editorial
FROM OUR EDITOR
Editorial
FROM OUR GUEST EDITOR
Interview
FAF DU PLESSIS
Sports Science
THE USE OF A CLINICAL TRIAD IN DIAGNOSING PERIPHERAL NERVE COMPRESSIONS
Sports Radiology
IMAGING TECHNIQUES FOR PERIPHERAL NERVE COMPRESSIONS

Categories

Member of
Organization members